This
article is the second in a series of short pieces in which I consider
various views of human nature and the human situation as summarized in
John Lanchester’s interesting New Yorker magazine essay “Pursuing
Happiness”
(see also my intro article on his
essay).
Lanchester
begins with our early homo sapiens
ancestors. Living under
constant threat in a hard and hostile environment, these ancestors were
exposed
to evolutionary pressures selecting for an emphasis on possible
negative
outcomes rather than a sunny, optimistic disposition (which got people
killed).
So the conclusion Lanchester offers, based in part on the work of
authors such
as Jonathon Haidt, is that happiness doesn’t come easily to us, because
we are
descendants of the surviving worriers, and are genetically disposed to
emphasize
the negative. And we do this on a pre-conscious level, reacting before
we’ve
had any time to assess the apparent threat or to consider alternatives.
In
fact, the evolutionarily-determined bias goes even further … Lanchester
quotes an important point made by Haidt, that in evolutionary terms,
“bad is
stronger than good. … Responses to threats and unpleasantness are
faster,
stronger, and harder to inhibit than responses to opportunities and
pleasures.”
Lanchester adds that since our brains are wired to respond to sense
data with
fight-or-flight reactions before passing the data on to higher cortical
centers, we’re reacting before we even consciously know what we’re
reacting to.
Piet and I will return to consider this point often in various WoK
pieces.
I
have great admiration for our early ancestors, making a life in the
midst of many uncertainties and in the face of great difficulties. And
I also
admire science for providing us with knowledge about our pre-historic
ancestry, a time we have no direct way of accessing. We may never
exhaust the
benefits
this gift confers for self-understanding, medicine, etc. But claims
based on
this kind of indirect access have often been overstated, and it’s also
important
to use this and all gifts from science carefully, and in the context of
other
ways of knowing about ourselves … ways that are
more direct.
First
of all, let’s accept that evolution shaped us both physically and
mentally, where this shaping includes a lot of neurologically “hard
wired”
reactions that by-pass our capacity for higher-level reflective
thought. I’ll
also accept the notion that a certain degree of wariness was an
evolutionarily
adaptive trait, although I doubt that it should be directly equated
with an
unhappy disposition (or that an inclination to contentment amounts to a
dangerously
reckless or happy-go-lucky one). Finally, it’s easy to imagine that Ig
and Og
were at the mercy of circumstances, dependent for survival on what
happened in
the world around them.
Granting
most of what Lanchester suggests so far, I still doubt that the
Ig’s and Og’s lacked crucial knowledge about how things tended to
happen (they
didn’t live in constant perplexity), or that they were exposed to
constant and
unpredictable threats to such a degree that happiness was precluded. In
fact I
suspect that they (even Lanchester’s circumspect caveman Og) found much
in life
to be satisfying. An evolutionary picture is actually consistent with
that
view, since Ig's and Og's evolved
in their environment, they weren't just subjected to it! Ig and Og might
even have been good at staying in the “flow” Lanchester
touts late in his article.
Being
wary (circumspect, cautious) was doubtless important in Og’s
time, as it is now, but I think even more important was and is the
capacity to
be sensitivelyconnected
to
things,
to be a part of them in some way, and to respond appropriately, rather
than
just reacting. Without that capacity, I doubt that “a tendency to
emphasize the
negative” would, by itself, have sufficed as an adaptive trait. I think
it
might even have been maladaptive for our species (it might be enough
for some
types of simpler organisms, but then they would have been more
restricted to particular
environments than homo
sapiens
have been and are now, on
various levels WoK will discuss).
I
don’t know whether evolutionary psychologists have done any work on my
latter claim yet, but I think in some form it will hold up. We’ll see …
the
reason I feel comfortable maintaining it, is that contemplative
traditions
have
found that a capacity to be exquisitely connected to our defining
contexts is much
more available and fundamental to our nature than we usually believe
nowadays. And
even by evolutionary reasoning, if we can do it, so must our
ancestors—Ig and
Og.
No
matter how carefully we apply evolutionary studies, and how refined our
knowledge gleaned from such studies might be, it would be a mistake to
take
evolutionary psychology as the only way of exploring some of the basic
issues
Lanchester raises. We have other ways of knowing about ourselves, and
they show
a complementary picture (I would never expect it to stand in real
conflict with
science or evolution) that offers different insights regarding our
human nature,
cognitive limitations, the degree to which the latter are “fixed,” and
the
nature of happiness. This is all by way of broaching the
question of
what is true about us, and what is the reality that contextualizes,
defines, satisfies and edifies
us.
In
my next article in this series, I’ll begin to introduce some
of what's involved in such alternate—and
more “direct” views of these issues.